Power, optimism, and risk-taking

نویسندگان

  • CAMERON ANDERSON
  • ADAM D. GALINSKY
چکیده

Five studies investigated the hypotheses that the sense of power increases optimism in perceiving risks and leads to more risky behavior. In Studies 1 and 2, individuals with a higher generalized sense of power and those primed with a high-power mind-set were more optimistic in their perceptions of risk. Study 3 primed the concept of power nonconsciously and found that both power and gain/loss frame had independent effects on risk preferences. In Study 4, those primed with a high-power mind-set were more likely to act in a risk-seeking fashion (i.e., engage in unprotected sex). In Study 5, individuals with a higher sense of power in a face-to-face negotiation took more risks by divulging their interests. The effects of power on risk-taking were mediated by optimistic risk perceptions and not by selfefficacy beliefs. Further, these effects were attenuated when the high-power individual felt a sense of responsibility. Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Power fascinates. People spend an inordinate amount of time attending to, thinking about, and discussing the thoughts and behaviors of powerful and prestigious individuals—be they heads of state, CEOs, or prominent members of their local church, club, or community (Chance, 1967; Fiske, 1993; Hall, 1984; Keltner & Robinson, 1997). Though typically viewed as frivolous and the province of gossip and gawking, this interest in powerholders is often important and useful. On a practical level, understanding the minds of those with power helps people appreciate how their leaders make decisions—decisions that impact people’s own lives. The behaviors of the powerful have inordinate pull, in that their actions have greater impact and matter more compared to those without power. On a theoretical level, understanding powerholders’ behavior can also provide a window into human nature more broadly; for only when people possess power do some of their deepest desires and motivations reveal themselves in the light of day. Research on the possession of power has shown that power affects diverse psychological processes, from stereotyping (Fiske, 1993) to styles of dress (Pfeffer, 1992). To help integrate these disparate findings, a recent theory proposed that power influences the relative activation of two broad and fundamental behavioral systems: the behavioral approach and inhibition systems (Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003). As we discuss below, these two behavioral systems help individuals pursue Received 28 June 2004 Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 22 September 2005 *Correspondence to: Cameron P. Anderson, University of California, Walter A. Haas School of Business, 545 Student Services Bldg #1900, Berkeley, CA 94720-1900, USA. E-mail: [email protected] rewards and avoid threats, respectively, by coordinating diverse affective, cognitive, and behavioral processes. Although a number of recent studies have begun to provide empirical support for this theory (Anderson & Berdahl, 2002; Galinsky, Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003), many of its tenets and implications have yet to be directly examined. In the current investigation, we explore the implications of this Approach/Inhibition model by examining the effects of possessing power on risk perceptions and risk-taking behavior. Intuitively, it might seem that lacking power would be associated with more risky behaviors, and there is some empirical evidence to support this intuition. For example, low levels of socioeconomic status (SES) have been associated with higher rates of risky sexual behavior, drug use, and behavioral habits (Adler et al., 1994; Capaldi, Stoolmiller, Clark, & Owen et al., 2002; Marmot, Shipley, & Rose, 1984). Social exclusion has been shown to produce more risky, self-defeating behaviors (Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2002). Some additional evidence emerges from primate studies in which lower-status male vervet monkeys tend to be more impulsively violent compared to their higher status peers (Fairbanks, Melega, Jorgensen, Kaplan, & McGuire, 2001). In fact, this violent impulsivity is one mechanism by which a low-status monkey can climb the status hierarchy. Thus, low-power individuals might be more risk-seeking because they are willing to do anything to get out of their disadvantaged position. This notion also brings into sharp relief the fact that low-power individuals have less to lose by behaving in a risky manner. The powerful may lose access to and control over valued resources if the downside of risk is realized, and thus the powerful might fear losing what they have (i.e., their gains), and act more conservatively. This logic of the powerless having less to lose by taking risks would also appear to be consistent with prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), which states that being in domain of losses produces risk-seeking behavior. If the lack of power puts people in the domain of losses and possessing power puts people into the domain of gains, then power should have a negative relationship with risky behavior. However, based on the idea that possessing and lacking power differentially activate the behavioral approach and inhibition systems, we propose that power increases, rather than decreases, risk-taking behavior. As we will argue, possessing power should lead individuals to pay more attention to the potential payoffs inherent in risky actions and devote less attention to the potential dangers. And as we detail in Study 3, the value function of prospect theory is conceptually independent of the optimism people feel toward potential outcomes, and therefore our hypotheses are independent of those from prospect theory. Therefore, power should increase optimism when perceiving risks, which should lead to an increased propensity to engage in risks. We tested the path from power to optimism to risky behavior in five studies, using multiple instantiations of the sense of power, and multiple measures of optimism, risk perception, and risk preference. Across these studies, we examined whether power leads to more optimistic risk perceptions and to a preference for riskier paths of action, regardless of how the sense of power is activated or assessed (semantically, through a recall task, with an individual difference measure, or a context-specific measure), or the nature of the risk involved (minor or major, relevant to self or not, controllable or uncontrollable). Further, we aimed to rule out alternative explanations for the findings, such as self-efficacy based accounts, and to identify boundary conditions—specifically, we aimed to show that when powerful individuals feel a sense of responsibility they become less risk-seeking. POWER, APPROACH, AND INHIBITION Power is often defined as the capacity to influence others and it primarily stems from the control over valuable resources and the ability to administer rewards and punishments (Emerson, 1962; French & 512 Cameron Anderson and Adam D. Galinsky Copyright # 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 36, 511–536 (2006) Raven, 1959; Goldhamer & Shils, 1939; Keltner et al., 2003; Lewin, 1951). Thus, power is a relational variable, in that individuals’ power can be understood only in relation to another person or a group (Emerson, 1962; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). According to the Approach/Inhibition Theory of power, possessing power triggers the relative activation of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems (Carver & White, 1994; Fowles, 1980; Gray, 1982, 1987, 1991; Higgins, 1997, 1998; Newman, 1997; Sutton & Davidson, 1997). The behavioral approach system is posited to regulate behavior associated with rewards, such as food, achievement, sex, safety, and social attachment. For example, positive emotion motivates approachrelated behavior, scanning for rewards in the environment, and forward locomotion (Carver & White, 1994; Gray, 1994; Higgins, 1997, 1998). The behavioral inhibition system has been equated to an alarm system. Once activated by threats or potential punishments, this system triggers affective states such as anxiety, heightened vigilance for threats in the environment, and avoidance and response inhibition (Gray, 1991; Higgins, 1997). Having high power is posited to activate the approach system for two reasons. First, elevated power is associated with increased access to rewards. When people have power they have access to more material resources such as financial resources and physical comforts, as well as social resources such as higher esteem, praise, and positive attention (Buss, 1996; Chance, 1967; Derber, 1979; Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1989; Ellis, 1993; French & Raven, 1959; Keltner, Young, Heerey, Oemig, & Monarch, 1998; Mazur, 1973; Operario & Fiske, 2001; Savin-Williams, 1979; Weisfeld, 1993). Second, when people have power they encounter less interference from others when pursuing rewards (Keltner et al., 1998; Weber, 1947; Winter & Stewart, 1983). For complementary reasons, having low power is proposed to activate the inhibition system. When people have low power they are subject to more social and material threats, especially the threat of losing favor among the powerful (e.g., Anderson & Berdahl, 2002; Chance, 1967; Fiske, 1993; Hall & Halberstadt, 1994; Whitney & Smith, 1993), and they are acutely aware of the constraints the these threats place upon their behavior (Anderson, John, & Keltner, 2005; Keltner et al., 2003). A number of recent studies have begun to provide support for the Approach/Inhibition Theory. Individuals randomly assigned to high-power conditions through a variety of means have been shown to pay more attention to positive and rewarding information, experience more positive affect, express themselves more freely in social interactions, pursue rewards more assertively, and show to be more of an action-orientation toward the material and social environment compared to individuals in lowpower conditions (Anderson & Berdahl, 2002; Galinsky et al., 2003; Langner et al., 2005; Smith & Bargh, 2005).

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

Optimistic Risk Perception in the Temporal Difference error Explains the Relation between Risk-taking, Gambling, Sensation-seeking and Low Fear

Understanding the affective, cognitive and behavioural processes involved in risk taking is essential for treatment and for setting environmental conditions to limit damage. Using Temporal Difference Reinforcement Learning (TDRL) we computationally investigated the effect of optimism in risk perception in a variety of goal-oriented tasks. Optimism in risk perception was studied by varying the c...

متن کامل

Determinants of Risk Taking Behavior: The role of Risk Attitudes, Risk Perceptions and Beliefs

Our study analyzes the determinants of investors’ risk taking behavior. We find that investors’ risk taking behavior such as portfolio choices can be predicted using risk attitudes, risk perceptions and belief measures such as optimism and overconfidence. However, the predictive power of these determinants heavily depends on the domain in which they were elicited. More specifically, risk attitu...

متن کامل

خاستگاه خطرپذیری اقتصادی در جوانان ایرانی: احتیاج یا طمع؟

Risk taking is an important field in economic neuroscience. The purpose of present study is evaluation of risk taking in three conditions of high requirement, low requirement and no requirement statement and correlation of it to cultural values. In this cross sectional case study, 391 Iranian youth were evaluated with an implicit risk taking task and cultural dimensions questionnaire. Repetit...

متن کامل

Board Compensation and Risk-Taking: The Moderating Role of CEO Duality (Evidence from Banking Industry)

  The purpose of this paper is to explore relationship between board compensation and risk taking with regard to CEO duality in the banking industry. Using a panel data regression model, with regard to optimal contracting and managerial power theory, we examined the data to determine the relationship between board compensation and risk taking of twenty one banks, for the period 2012 to 2018. R...

متن کامل

Guilt enhances the sense of control and drives risky judgments.

In the present studies, we investigate the hypothesis that guilt influences risk taking by enhancing one's sense of control. Across multiple inductions of guilt, we demonstrate that experimentally induced guilt enhances optimism about risks for the self (Study 1), preferences for gambles versus guaranteed payoffs (Studies 2, 4, and 6), and the likelihood that one will engage in risk-taking beha...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2012